BW: So the brain is about survival …
JL: Yeah, that’s its main job, to keep us alive. But it’s not just about biological survival. It’s also about social and cultural survival. We are in social situations all the time. We have to figure out who’s going to benefit us, who’s going to harm us. You can’t say the wrong thing to your boss. You are going to a party where there will be social challenges to navigate, whether it’s a work party, fun party or whatever. There are lots of things that our brain has to manage that are not just about surviving as a wild animal but surviving in our modern sociocultural environment.
BW: Can you locate the parts of the brain that are involved with social survival?
JL: There’s a lot of work now in social neuroscience. The challenge there, when they’re doing experiments in laboratory as opposed to real-life situations, is what does it mean to have a social stimulus. For example, a face is thought to be a social stimulus. A fearful face will activate the amygdala. But it turns out that if you really look at what it is about the face that is activating the amygdala, it’s mostly around the eyes. And you can even activate the amygdala if all you show on the screen is the shape of the whites of the eyes in a fearful situation.
BW: Really? I guess that’s where that expression comes from — whites of the eyes!
JL: It’s a social stimulus because it’s another human, but it’s not the whole person. Very basic primitive stimuli shoot to the amygdala and activate it. You’re not conscious of this. You’re not aware that it’s the whites of the eyes. The amygdala “sees” the specific trigger. It goes in there, releases hormones, and all of the sudden consciously you’re paying attention to the environment and you see the face of the person. But it’s the eyes that did the deed. A lot of stuff is triggered by very simple stimuli in our social encounters, and then we elaborate it through cognitive and conscious processes.
BW: You’re walking along, you see a snake, it becomes a memory and the next time you’re at that spot you have that fear …
JL: You’re talking about three kinds of memory. One is memory in the conventional sense, which is your conscious experience. You remember where you saw that snake, you remember seeing that snake. But the way your brain learned to be defensive about that situation is that information not only went to the part of the brain that formed the conscious memory, which is called the hippocampus, but also to the amygdala. The amygdala detected the danger in the snake, because the snake is more or less an innate stimulus for people. Not everyone has a snake phobia, but we all have some kind of an innate response to snakes. You might freeze when you see the snake; that will trigger the release of hormones; hormones come back to the brain where they imprint the amygdala and hippocampal memories and make them stronger.
The next time you go back to that place, although there’s no snake, you’re now dealing with the context. The amygdala is important not only in detecting the snake but in detecting the context. It’s the contextual information coming into the amygdala that then triggers the fear response again: the release of hormones, brain arousal, and all sorts of things that are facilitating the retrieval of the conscious declarative memories about the past experience. So you’ve got the cognitive memories now in your mind supplemented by the arousal that the unconscious amygdala memories are generating at the same time, and those are coming together to make that conscious memory.
BW: What is the goal of your research?
JL: To help people who have problems with fear, because activation of the defense system is a major factor in people who have fear and anxiety disorders. They’re perpetually in that state, or that state is easily triggered. The defense circuit contributes in a major way. Maybe it’s off-kilter, and that’s why one has problems. We’re still trying to understand how fear and anxiety come about, but we’re saying that one of the components is not a fear system or anxiety system but a defense system. It’s doing the same thing regardless of what we call it, but it clarifies what the role of this thing is, especially from an evolutionary point of view. It’s not in the brain to create feelings like fear and anxiety. It’s in the brain to allow an organism, whether it be a bacterium or a human, to detect and respond to danger.
BW: Are there ways to stop disorders like post-traumatic stress disorder?
JL: One of the things we work on is how to alter the memories that are created in these situations. We reported a discovery in a paper in Nature, something called “reconsolidation.” Basically, when you retrieve a memory, the memory has to go through a new learning process in order to be kept in the brain. A key thing involved in formation of memory is that the neurons forming the memory have to synthesize new proteins. These stabilize the synaptic connections that are constituting the memory. Memory is thought of as a set of synaptic inputs that have changed as a result of some experience, and the way those changes are stabilized is by protein synthesis. If you block protein synthesis after you retrieve a memory, you lose the memory just as you do if you block protein synthesis while you’re forming the memory.
The idea there is that each time you retrieve a memory you have to put it back in, because it is now a new memory. You have to re-store it, that’s why it’s called reconsolidation. So a person goes to court to testify about a crime they witnessed and talks about what they read in the newspaper, rather than what they gave in the police report on the day of the crime, because the memory was reactivated by reading the newspaper. The new info gets incorporated in the memory and is stored as part of the new memory, and that’s what the person remembers. The bottom line is that our memory is only as good as our last memory. Not very reliable, but people have been saying that for a long time. Somehow it all works out most of the time.
It does raise questions about our legal system. I’ve often been asked to give expert testimony on murder crimes, and I always turn those things down. “I didn’t do it, my amygdala did.” First of all, if it’s about memory, it’s a slippery slope, and I’d rather stay out of it. I have, on occasion, given information saying that memory is unreliable, but not in a formal setting. I think the courts are aware of this; they don’t know what to do about it.
This article was originally published in the Fall 2012 issue of Brain World Magazine.